Partager

Centre pour la Recherche et l'Innovation dans l'Enseignement - CERI

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Primer

 

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Primer

by

Petra Hurks, PhD

The purpose of this document is to answer frequently asked questions regarding ADHD and to point the reader towards further resources on the subject.

Petra Hurks is an assistant professor at Maastricht University, The Netherlands. She has appointments at both the Faculty of Medicine and the Faculty of Psychology of this university, spending half her time in each Fac-ulty. In addition, her research has focused on the etiology, developmental profile and treatment of attention disorders, and more specifically Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (abbreviated ADHD).

Definition of ADHD

Over the past centuries, the condition presently known as Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) underwent many transformations in terminology and definition. Today, ADHD is defined as a persistent pat-tern of inattention, hyperactivity and/or impulsivity that is, on average, more frequent and severe than that observed in individuals at a comparable level of development. In addition, this behavioural pattern has led to at least some impairment in social, academic, or occupational functioning. Because almost all children will show some of the predefined characteristics at some stage in their life, one can only speak of ADHD under strict conditions, including definitions related to the duration of symptomatology (e.g. the behaviour has to manifest itself over a period of at least 6 months), severity (e.g. the behaviour has to be present in at least two domains, for instance at home and at school), and recognition of the abnormal behaviour by both the teacher and the parent. Finally, the symptoms do not occur exclusively during the course of a Pervasive Developmen-tal Disorder, Schizophrenia, or other Psychotic Disorder and are not better accounted for by another mental disorder (e.g. Mood Disorder, Anxiety Disorder, Dissociative Disorder, or a Personality Disorder) (APA, 1994). 

Are there different types of ADHD?

Yes. A ‘pure ADHD’ group is heterogeneous. ADHD can be divided into three subtypes: [1] ADHD predomi-nantly inattentive type (i.e. individuals who primarily have problems in concentrating, focusing and sustaining their attention), [2] ADHD predominantly hyperactive-impulsive type (i.e. individuals who are mainly rest-less, have problems in staying still, etc.), and (3) ADHD combined type (i.e. individuals who score high on inattention as well as hyperactivity and impulsivity) (APA, 1994). It is highly likely that the behaviour or symp-toms of children suffering from the predominantly inattentive type of ADHD is different from that of children with the predominantly hyperactive-impulsive type or the combined type. For instance, researchers found greater academic underachievement in children with ADHD of the predominantly inattentive type than in children with the predominantly hyperactive-impulsive type (e.g., Morgan, Hynd, Riccio, & Hall, 1996).

What is the prevalence of ADHD?

ADHD is one of the most frequently monitored developmental syndromes worldwide and is known to occur in several cultures. Prevalence estimates fluctuate between 1% and 20%, with an average of 3-5% for the general population (Barkley, 1998; Kroes et al., 2001), depending on how strictly criteria are applied, cultural differences, and the degree of consensus between caregivers, teachers, and physicians.

Do boys differ from girls in terms of ADHD symptoms?

Both in clinical and epidemiological samples, it has been found that ADHD is much more common in boys than in girls – 9 to 1 in clinical samples and 4 to 1 in the general population (Cantwell, 1996). Today, the accuracy of these estimates is being questioned. As shown recently, girls seem to have primarily attention and cognition problems and fewer of aggression/impulsivity; this latter would probably lead to an earlier referral (Abikoff et al., 2002).

Does ADHD co-occur with other disorders?

Yes. Comorbidity has been revealed to be an important consideration when dealing with a child suffering from ADHD. For example, in 50-80% of the cases, ADHD occurs simultaneously with other psychiatric or developmental problems (reviewed by Biederman, Newcorn, & Sprich, 1991; Jensen, Martin, & Cantwell, 1997), although the actual prevalence of comorbidity may vary across different types of samples, depending on whether the sample is clinical (paediatric or psychiatric) or drawn from a general population (Busch et al., 2002). The most important comorbid syndromes and/or co-occurring disorders are:

(i)  Oppositional Defiant Disorder and Conduct Disorder (at school age estimates range from 40-90%)
(ii)  Anxiety (in approximately 25% of the cases) and Mood Disorders (15-20%)
(iii)  Learning Disabilities (20%)
(iv)  Minor forms of Pervasive Developmental Disorders
(v)  Gilles de la Tourette syndrome
(vi)  Developmental Coordination Disorder.

Comorbidity often has consequences for the diagnostic process and can have an impact on the natural history of ADHD and on the success of treatment programs. For instance, several authors have stated that the cogni-tive deficits and the outcomes (e.g. drug use and driving-related accidents) for children with concurrent ADHD and Conduct Disorder are worse than those of children with ADHD or those of children with Conduct Disorders alone. Assessment and treatment of comorbid disorders are, therefore, often as important as assess-ment and treatment of ADHD (Prins & Ten Brink, 2000; Tannock, 1998).

Will my child “grow out of” ADHD?

In contrast to earlier beliefs, follow-up studies have consistently documented ADHD symptoms as persisting throughout adolescence and young adulthood. Although relatively little is known about ADHD children as young adults, the information that does exist suggests that at least 60% of the children with ADHD will con-tinue to have symptoms of ADHD as they grow up (Barkley, 1996). The problems that adults have to face as a consequence of earlier ADHD symptoms in childhood range from residual ADHD symptoms that impair home or work adjustment, to depression, substance abuse, low self-esteem, accident proneness, and anti-social personality (Weiss & Trokenberg Hechtman, 1993; Wender, Reimherr, & Wood, 1981).

What causes ADHD?

Today, it is still not completely clear what exactly causes ADHD. However, it is believed that no single factor can explain all cases of the clinical syndrome ‘ADHD’ and that more likely an interaction of several factors (e.g. biological and environmental and psychological) leads to ADHD (Tannock, 1998; Weiss, 1996).

Can ADHD be inherited?

If there is a genetic component in ADHD, the syndrome should be more prevalent among biological relatives of a child with ADHD than among relatives of control subjects. Over the past decades, several studies have reported that ADHD is diagnosed at least 5 times more often among the relatives of ADHD children than among other families. In addition, high rates of disorders or syndromes, such as behavioural disorders (like conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder), anxiety disorders, and mood disorders, were found among first-degree relatives of individuals suffering from ADHD (e.g., Biederman et al., 1986). Also, twin and adoption stud-ies support the hypothesis that there is a genetic component in ADHD, even after controlling for environmental factors (e.g., Morrison & Stewart, 1971; Van der Oord, Boomsma, & Verhulst, 1994). However, caution in interpreting these results is warranted, for several reasons. For one, ADHD is found to occur simultaneously with other psychiatric or developmental problems in 50-80% of the cases. Comings et al. (2000) reported that some of these co-occurring disorders or syndromes also have a genetic component. Therefore, in children with both ADHD and other disabilities or syndromes, the genetic effects may be due to the presence of the co-occurring symptoms in the group and not so much due to ADHD. However, after controlling for comorbidity, consistent evidence still points toward an increased prevalence of both childhood and adulthood ADHD in first-degree and second-degree relatives of ADHD probands, a relation that applies to both males and females (reviewed by Faraone & Biederman, 1994).

Is there a neurobiological basis for ADHD?

Yes. In the case of ADHD, imaging techniques point toward differences between individuals with ADHD and without ADHD in terms of the structure and functioning of specific parts of the brain (e.g. prefrontal cortex, caudate nucleus, corpus callosum, basal ganglia, and cerebellum). Caution in the interpretation of the neuro-anatomical evidence is, however, still warranted, because of sample bias (e.g. small sample sizes, sex differ-ences, limited data on neuropsychological characteristics and handedness of the participants), inconsistencies in findings for both normal controls and children with ADHD, etc.

Cognitive deficits in ADHD?

In school-aged children, cognitive deficits are hypothesised as being a core part of ADHD symptomatology and are thought to play a major role in the difficult adaptation of children suffering from this disorder (Seidman, Biederman, Faraone, Weber, & Ouelette, 1997). As a group, these children tend to exhibit sub-average or a relatively poor performance on various classical cognitive tests measuring both input-related and output-related information processes. For example, a deficiency was found in sustained attention or vigilance, as observed by an in-creased variability in test performance and a decreased accuracy (Barkley, 1998). In addition, children with ADHD had a poorer performance on several aspects of executive functioning (such as organisation and com-plex problem solving), non-verbal working memory, internalisation of speech, self-monitoring and self-regulation (e.g., Barkley, 1998; Perugini, Harvey, Lovejoy, Sandstrom, & Webb, 2000).

Examples of current theories that relate cognitive functioning and ADHD are (1) Barkley’s disinhibition the-ory, (2) the theory of a time perception deficit in ADHD, and (3) the cognitive-energetic model. Barkley’s disinhibition theory states that the construct of impulsiveness (i.e. poor behavioural inhibition) is a central deficit in ADHD. Children suffering from the disorder fail to inhibit or delay a behavioural response, which secondarily causes the observed deficits in executive functioning and motor output (Barkley, 1998; Barkley, Edwards, Laneri, Fletcher, & Metevia, 2001). Another theory concerning a cognitive deficit in ADHD is associated with time perception. Several authors have stated that a deficit in time estimation or an altered sense of time may explain several problems related to ADHD, such as problems with waiting, delaying responses, and with delaying gratification (e.g., Rubia et al., 2001; Smith, Taylor, Rogers, Newman, & Rubia, 2002; Sonuga-Barke, Saxton, & Hall, 1998). A third theory is the cognitive-energetic model of information processing. This model states that the fundamental problem in ADHD is not so much a structural cognitive limitation (as stated in, for instance, Barkley’s model) but more an energetic limitation, and that the key symptoms of ADHD (i.e. poor attention capacity, restlessness, and a lack of impulse control) are mainly caused by state factors, such as boredom, time-on-task, reward, and exter-nal control (e.g., Borcherding et al., 1988; Borger & Van der Meere, 2000). The outcome of empirical research on this issue has been less consistent than expected.

How to diagnose ADHD?

It is still very difficult to reliably diagnose ADHD; for one because the main clusters of symptoms (inatten-tion, hyperactivity, impulsive behaviour) are human traits that can be observed in the behavioural pattern of every individual during his/her lifespan. However, individuals suffering from ADHD display this natural be-haviour in a more extreme manner and their behaviour often leads to impediments in daily functioning. An-other point worth mentioning here is that there is no objective instrument presently available that is sensitive and specific enough to support a diagnosis of ADHD with a 100% certainty.
Presently, the diagnosis is often made by a clinician after a systematic evaluation of data collected by use of objective tests, interviews with caregivers and teachers, and outcomes of scientific studies. Relevant informa-tion collected by the clinicians in order to diagnose ADHD is for instance:
• Extended systematic evaluation of the ADHD criteria by including data from both the caregivers as well as the teacher of the child. Multiple informers (e.g. teachers as well as parents) are required for several reasons. First, caregivers are believed to overestimate externalising behaviour in their chil-dren, while teachers are able to compare the behaviour of the child with the norm. Secondly, when a child only reveals overactivity, impulsiveness, or attention problems in one situation, it may be that the behaviour can be explained by some factor other than ADHD (e.g. an unstructured, chaotic envi-ronment).
• Testing for the pervasiveness of the behavioural characteristics, in which it is claimed that the devia-tions in behaviour are present in more than 1 situation over a period of more than 6 months.
• Evaluation of comorbidity (such as learning disorders, Conduct Disorder, affective disorders, tic dis-orders). As mentioned earlier, in a high percentage of cases, children with ADHD have co-occurring syndromes or disorders, which could lead to specific characteristics. Comorbidity often has conse-quences for the diagnostic process and can have an influence on the natural history of the disorder and on the success of treatment programs. Assessment and treatment of comorbid disorders is often e-qually as important as that of ADHD (Prins & Ten Brink, 2000; Tannock, 1998).
• Collection of data about the development and current functioning of the child. Because ADHD is a developmental syndrome, it is necessary to obtain not only information concerning behaviour in the past, but also medical data (such as head trauma, diseases), family history of ADHD, etc. In addition, certain disorders or disabilities can cause a behavioural pattern similar to that of ADHD. After treating these disorders it is assumed the behaviour of the child will change. Therefore, it is necessary to con-trol for these factors when diagnosing ADHD.
• Does the ADHD symptomatology lead to dysfunction at school, home, or socially? And to what ex-tent is the family affected by ADHD?

It takes, thereby, time and experience to diagnose ADHD, and the diagnosis is still uncertain. Finally, the di-agnosis of ‘ADHD’ can be both liberating and restricting for a child, its caregivers, and the social environ-ment. Therefore, clinicians should be cautious in making this diagnosis, and it should only be made as a guide for interventional strategies.

How to treat ADHD?

After diagnosing ADHD, the clinician has to choose the most optimal treatment for the child suffering from ADHD. Unfortunately, none of the existing therapies for treating ADHD make the syndrome disappear com-pletely and forever. The goal of the therapies currently available is to decrease the seriousness of symptoma-tology, in order to improve the child’s functioning at school and at home. Treatment can include several ele-ments, e.g. pharmacological and non-pharmacological therapy, psychological interventions (e.g. giving care-givers and educators additional insight into the cause, seriousness, outcome, and treatment of ADHD, behav-ioural therapy, and training of social interactions). Frequently, several of these elements are successfully com-bined while treating the child with ADHD.

Medication

Treatment of ADHD by use of medication is successful in mediating several aspects of behaviour in 80-90% of the children suffering from ADHD; e.g. with regard to concentration abilities, activity levels, impulsive behaviour, aggressiveness, and school performance and learning. A disadvantage of this type of treatment is, however, that the positive effects of medication disappear when the use of medication is stopped. In addition, most medications can have at least minor side effects, such as heart palpitations, agitation, nausea, a decreased appetite, weight loss, and sleeping problems.   

Psychological intervention for the caregiver

For caregivers, it is often extremely difficult to raise a child suffering from ADHD. Because of the child’s behaviour, it is often difficult for the caregiver to stay calm and positive. This is unfortunate, for research has shown that children with ADHD profit most from an environment that always reacts in a positive manner when the child displays positive and correct behaviour. Caregivers may benefit from psychological interven-tions provided individually or within a group. The main goal of psychological interventions is to provide the caregiver with skills that can be implemented in daily family life.

Psychological interventions designed for caregivers serve a number of purposes, e.g.:
• For one, the therapist provides the caregiver with correct and detailed information about ADHD [e.g. about the prognosis, causes, and what to be expected of the child]. This is called psycho-education.
• Furthermore, the caregiver is taught how to create a predictable, structured environment for the child, in order to inculcate various coping skills, suitable study habits and a sense of discipline and responsi-bility into children. The caregiver is, for instance, taught that it is necessary to be consistent in his/her reactions towards the child. It is important to explain to the child what is allowed and what is not, and what the consequences of his or her actions are. The same rule holds at all times; the child is consis-tently punished or awarded immediately after doing something wrong or right. If a reward or punish-ment is delayed, the child will be unable to comprehend why. In line with this, research has revealed that ADHD children react positively to a so-called ‘response-cost’ principle. This is a method in which ADHD children gain points when they act correctly and lose points when they do something wrong. If the child collects a certain amount of points, he or she can exchange these points for a pre-defined reward (e.g. a trip to the zoo or cinema).

Psychological intervention for the teacher

It is of extreme importance that the teacher of the child suffering from ADHD cooperates with the therapist and the caregiver. The teacher is taught how to create a predictable, structured environment for the child, in order to inculcate into him/her various coping skills, suitable study habits and a sense of discipline and re-sponsibility. A frequently used method is the ‘daily report card’. The teacher is asked to report on this card how a child has functioned within the classroom during a predefined teaching period (e.g. during the entire day, during the morning or the afternoon). Scoring can be done on, for instance: the ability to sit still, the abil-ity to listen to other children or the teacher, the ability to finish school work, quality of school work, punctual-ity. At the end of the day, the child takes the report card home. The child is awarded or punished for his or her performance at school that day (e.g. the child is allowed to play a computer game for 30 minutes or receives points). Introducing ‘time out periods’, seating the ADHD child in the front row of the classroom, and asking the child to do the tasks that demand attention in the early part of the day, etc. have also been proven effective. By doing this, the child is less distracted and able to perform better.

Psychological intervention for the child

One of the major problems of children suffering from ADHD, is their impulsive behaviour (i.e. they fre-quently act before thinking). Also, these children are often unstructured, and have difficulties in planning their activities. It is, therefore, important that the child learns to control his or her own behaviour and to think first before acting. A method frequently used in this respect is ‘Stop-Think-Do’. During group or individual ses-sions, the child learns to act according to a sequel of five pre-defined steps. In step 1, the child has to identify what the main problem is or work out what he or she has been asked to do. In step 2, the child has to think of several possible solutions. In step 3, the child has to select the most optimal solution from all the solutions considered during step 2. In step 4, the child has to think through which actions are necessary to carry out the most optimal solution chosen during step 3 and he or she has to try them out. In step 5, the child has to decide whether the solution selected in step 3 was the right one (i.e. evaluation).

See Also

PLoS Medicine: Essay on: "Medicine Goes to School: Teachers as Sickness Brokers for ADHD"

Internet sites for Teachers and Parents

www.chadd.org
www.adders.org
http://www.additudemag.com/
http://www.adhdnews.com/
http://www.addresources.org/directory_websites.php
www.addwarehouse.com

References

Abikoff, H. B., Jensen, P. S., Arnold, L. L. E., Hoza, B., Hechtman, L., Pollack, S., Martin, D., Alvir, J., March, J. S., Hinshaw, S., Vitiello, B., Newcorn, J., Greiner, A., Cantwell, D. P., Conners, C. K., Elliott, G., Greenhill, L. L., Kraemer, H., W.E., P. J., Severe, J. B., Swanson, J. M., Wells, K., & Wigal, T. (2002). Observed classroom behavior of children with ADHD: Relationship to gender and comorbidity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 349-359.

APA (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Barkley, R. A. (1996). Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Child psychopathology (pp. 63-112). New York: The Guilford press.

Barkley, R. A. (1998). Attention-Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment. (2 ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.

Barkley, R. A., Edwards, G., Laneri, M., Fletcher, K., & Metevia, L. (2001). Executive functioning, temporal discounting, and sense of time in adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD). Journal of abnormal Child Psychology, 29(6), 541-556.

Biederman, J., Munir, K., Knee, D., Habelow, W., Armentano, M., Autor, S., Hoge, S. K., & Waternaux, C. (1986). A family study of patients with attention deficit disorder and normal controls. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 20, 263-274.

Biederman, J., Newcorn, J., & Sprich, S. (1991). Comorbidity of attention defict hyperactivity disorder with conduct, depressive, anxiety and other disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 564-577.

Borcherding, B., Thompson, K., Kruesi, M., Bartko, J., Rapoport, J. L., & Weingartner, H. (1988). Automatic and effortful processing in Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16(3), 333-345.

Borger, N., & Van der Meere, J. (2000). Motor control and state regulation in children with ADHD: A cardiac response study. Biological Psychology, 51, 247-267.

Busch, B., Biederman, J., Glassner Cohen, L., Sayer, J. M., Monuteaux, M. C., Mick, E., Zallen, B., & Faraone, S. V. (2002). Correlates of ADHD among children in pediatric and psychiatric clinics. Psychiatric Services, 53, 1103-1111.

Cantwell, D. P. (1996). Attention Deficit Disorder: A review of the past 10 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 978-987.

Faraone, S. V., & Biederman, J. (1994). Genetics of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. In L. L. Greenhill (Ed.), Child and adolescent psychiatric clinics of North America: Disruptive disorders (Vol. 3, pp. 285-301).

Jensen, P. S., Martin, B. A., & Cantwell, D. P. (1997). Comorbidity in ADHD: Implications for research, practice, and DSM-IV. Journal of the American Academy Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1065-1079.

Kroes, M., Kalff, A. C., Kessels, A. G. H., Steyaert, J., Feron, F., Van Someren, A. J. W. G. M., Hurks, P. P. M., Hendriksen, J. G. M., Van Zeben, T. M. C. B., Rozendaal, N., Crolla, I. F. A. M., Troost, J., Jolles, J., & Vles, J. S. H. (2001). Child psychiatric diagnoses in a population of Dutch schoolchildren aged 6 to 8 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(12), 1401-1409.

Morgan, A. E., Hynd, G. W., Riccio, C. A., & Hall, J. (1996). Validity of DSM-IV ADHD Predominantly Inattentive and Combined Types: Relationship to previous DSM diagnoses/sybtype differences. Journal of the American Academy Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 325-333.

Morrison, J. R., & Stewart, M. A. (1971). A family study of the hyperactive child syndrome. Biological Psychiatry, 3, 189-195.

Perugini, E. M., Harvey, E. A., Lovejoy, D. W., Sandstrom, K., & Webb, A. H. (2000). The predictive power of combined neuropsychological measures for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in children. Child Neuropsychology, 6(2), 101-114.

Prins, P., & Ten Brink, E. (2000). Richtlijnen voor een multimodale behandeling van ADHD. In P. Prins & N. Pameijer (Eds.), Protocollen in de jeugdzorg: Richtlijnen voor diagnostiek, indicatiestelling en interventie. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.

Rubia, K., Taylor, A., Taylor, E., & Sergeant, J. A. (1999). Synchronization, anticipation, and consistency in motor timing of children with dimensionally defined attention deficit hyperactivity behaviour. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89, 1237-1358.

Rubia, K., Taylor, E., Smith, A., Oksannen, H., Overmeyer, S., Bullmore, E., & Newman, S. (2001). Neurophysiological analyses of impulsiveness in childhood hyperactivity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 138-143.

Seidman, L. J., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. v., Weber, W., & Ouelette, C. (1997). Toward defining a neuropsychology of Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder: Performance of children and adolescents from a large clinically referred sample. Journal of Consulting and Clinical psychology, 65(1), 150-160.

Smith, A., Taylor, E., Rogers, J. W., Newman, S., & Rubia, K. (2002). Evidence for a pure time perception deficit in children with ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(4), 529-542.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Saxton, T., & Hall, M. (1998). The role of interval underestimation in hyperactive children's failure to suppress responses over time. Behavioural Brain Research, 94, 45-50.

Stevens, D. A., Stover, C. E., & Backus, J. T. (1970). The hyperkinetic child: Effects of incentives on the speed of rapid tapping. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 56-59.

Tannock, R. (1998). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Advances in cognitive, neurobiological andgenetic research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 65-99.

Van der Oord, E. J. C. G., Boomsma, D. I., & Verhulst, F. C. (1994). A study of problem behaviros in 10- to 15-year old biologically related and unrelated international adoptees. Behavior Genetics, 24, 193-205.

Weiss, G. (1996). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In M. Lewis (Ed.), Child and adolescent psychiatry: A comprehensive textbook (pp. 544-563). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.

Weiss, G., & Trokenberg Hechtman, L. (1993). Hyperactive children grow up: ADHD in children , adolescents and adults. (2 ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.

Wender, P. H., Reimherr, F. W., & Wood, D. R. (1981). Attention deficit disorder ('minimal brain dysfunction') in adults. A replication study of diagnosis and drug treatment. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38(4), 449-456.

 

 

Documents connexes

 

Also AvailableEgalement disponible(s)